This post is part of a Reptile and Amphibian Blogging Network (RAmBlN) online event called #CrawliesConverge. We are writing on convergent evolution in reptiles and amphibians. Find our event schedule here. Follow on Twitter or Facebook.
Northern Pacific rattlesnake consuming a California ground squirrel pup. Photo: B.J. Putman |
Rattlesnakes and some other venomous snakes hunt in a stereotyped
manner called ambush hunting. They remain stationary at a hunting site,
patiently waiting for unsuspecting prey to wander by. They employ a rapid
strike, during which they embed their fangs into the prey’s body. They inject
venom into the prey, then typically release it and let it succumb to the venom before
ingesting it. Because most venomous ambush-hunting snakes around the world do
not drastically deviate from this general strategy, their prey experience almost
the same predation pressures. Thus, prey* have independently figured out
similar ways to avoid being eaten by ambush-hunting snakes.
*by prey, I’m talking about small mammals (squirrels,
gerbils, chipmunks, etc.). Snakes also consume other types of prey, but as far
as I know, these prey do not exhibit the same antisnake defenses as small
mammals.
Only a few research groups are studying the predator-prey
relationship between small mammals and snakes. The most well-known study systems
involve the Cape ground squirrel and cobras/puff adders in South Africa, the
kangaroo rat and rattlesnakes in the southwestern US, and the California ground
squirrel and rattlesnakes in California (my system!). However, we know many other small mammals
around the world exhibit similar defenses from anecdotal reports.
Left: a Cape ground squirrel harassing a cobra. Right: the squirrel leaps away from a strike. Photo: shockmansion.com |
Small mammals across the globe deter snake predation in
three main ways:
1) Sending Signals
Many small mammals are not scared by snakes. They boldly
approach them, investigate, and sometimes attack the snake! Typically, they will
also spend a considerable amount of time repeatedly moving a body appendage in
front of the snake. Most animals like squirrels and chipmunks wave their tails
at the snake (Kobayashi 1987, Hersek and Owings 1993, Clark 2005), while others, like kangaroo
rats and desert gerbils, drum their feet against the ground (Randall and Matocq 1997, Randall et al. 2000). Upon first glance, this
seems like a pretty dumb thing to do in front of an animal that could kill you.
However, these repetitive movements serve as warning signals to the snake, and
actually deter it from attacking. Research from our lab has shown that squirrel
tail-flagging tells the snake that it has been discovered (it has lost the
element of surprise), and that the squirrel is prepared for an attack (i.e. if
the snake strikes, it will likely miss) (Barbour and Clark 2012, Putman and Clark 2015). These signals could also inform
other small mammals in the area of the snake’s presence, further degrading the
value of its hunting site (because if everyone knows where the snake is,
everyone is going to avoid the area). Some suggest that small mammals wave
their tails to make themselves look like a larger more formidable opponent to
the snake, but no study has yet tested this hypothesis.
Video of a kangaroo rat footdrumming at a sidewinder rattlesnake in the Mojave Desert.
Snake in burrow. Rat starts drumming 0:53 sec into video.
Video from the Clark Lab YouTube Channel.
Snake in burrow. Rat starts drumming 0:53 sec into video.
Video from the Clark Lab YouTube Channel.
Video of a Cape ground squirrel and mongoose harassing a cobra. Both exhibit aerial leaps when startled.
Video from Smithsonian Channel.
2) Ninja Reactions
When small mammals signal to snakes that they are ready and
able to evade a strike, they are not lying. Thanks to snake predation, small
mammals around the world have become ninja warriors, able to rapidly escape sticky
situations using aerial acrobatics. This is a serious adaptation because a snake
can strike at a velocity of 4.5 m/sec, meaning it could take a snake less than
70 milliseconds to strike a prey 30 cm away! Small mammals need to use a response pathway that
bypasses cerebral processing in order to react fast enough. Hence, it has been
suggested that their responses to strikes are a type of startle response induced
by acoustic stimuli rather than visual (meaning they respond to the sound of a
snake strike and not the sight of one). The visual system responds via
G-protein-coupled receptors, which are too slow to induce the speed of response
we observe. The mechanoreceptors of the acoustic startle response are much
faster and bypass cerebral processing. One study supports this claim by showing
that only kangaroo rats with intact auditory systems were able to avoid rattlesnake
strikes, while experimentally deafened rats could not (Webster 1962).
A quick response is important in avoiding a snake strike, but so is the type of escape you use. Successfully outrunning a snake strike is unlikely (see velocity above) – you would literally need to be the Flash to survive. So instead of running away from strikes, small mammals have evolved the ability to leap vertically or horizontally away. This type of escape quickly propels the body of the small mammal away from the vector of a snake strike. Once snakes initiate an attack, their ability to alter their strike trajectory is limited, and so prey benefit more from displacing their bodies vertically or horizontally than by trying to outdistance the strike by moving within the same plane as the strike trajectory. Videos of these aerial leaps show that small mammals use their tails to contort themselves, often rotating their bodies near 180 degrees while midair. Evasive leaping is known to occur in squirrels, mongoose, and kangaroo rats. Kangaroo rats are the masters though, propelling themselves several body lengths upward when threatened with a strike.
Video showing the difference between squirrels running and leaping away from a
simulated rattlesnake strike (spring-loaded cork).
Video from Clark Lab YouTube Channel.
Video of a kangaroo rat leaping tremendously high in response to a strike.
Video from Clark Lab YouTube Channel.
3) Venom Resistance
Venom resistance levels of California ground squirrel blood paired with venom from different species of rattlesnake. Squirrels effectively inhibit the venom of C. oreganus and also C. v. viridis, a close relative of C. oreganus (gray bars). Resistance against C. atrox venom is low (red bar) because California squirrels are not generally preyed upon by this snake species. Taken from Biardi et al. 2011. |
Several species of small mammal are known to harbor
particularly high levels of venom resistance including the mongoose, opossum,
woodrat, woodchuck, and some species of ground squirrel (see Ovadia and Kochva 1977, Perez et al. 1978, Poran andCoss 1990, Biardi and Coss 2011, Jansa and Voss 2011). Interestingly, the Cape
ground squirrel which behaves similarly toward venomous snakes as the
California ground squirrel is not resistant to either of its snake predators,
the snouted cobra and the puff adder (Phillips et al. 2012). Findings like these help us
understand the constraints associated with evolving such defenses.
Many studies that have quantified venom resistance are
flawed because they do not compare snakes and small mammals from the same area.
For example, a study might examine how well squirrel blood inhibits venom by mixing
it with venom pooled from snake species across the country, not just the species
that only prey on the squirrels. Furthermore, if each population is in its own
evolutionary arms race, even using venom of the appropriate snake species may provide
us with an inaccurate measure of resistance – we have to use snakes and small
mammals from the exact same population because prey evolve specific blood
plasma properties to combat their local snakes, while snakes evolve specific
venom properties to overcome the local prey’s resistance. Matt Holding at Ohio State University is doing exactly this with the California ground squirrel-Pacific
rattlesnake system, and others are on their way to improving our knowledge on this topic.
rattlesnake system, and others are on their way to improving our knowledge on this topic.
***
Snakes not only remove prey individuals from a population
(rodent control), but impact their behaviors and physiology. The unique but
similar defenses small mammals have evolved against snake predation demonstrate
snakes’ importance as top level predators in ecosystems worldwide.
The infographic below summarizes this blog post - distribute at will :) :) :)
References:
The infographic below summarizes this blog post - distribute at will :) :) :)
References: